Formation: Examining Others in Depth "Considering The Effects of The Act"
Suppose a child knocked over and broke a cup. Is he fully
responsible for this act? We might want some additional information before
reaching a conclusion. Would it help if we knew that he had knocked down a
total of fifteen cups? Logically, it shouldn’t. Yet, in fact, we tend to
determine causality in part by the consequences of acts. After studying the
attribution of causality and intention in children, the Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget (1948) concluded that young
children seem to disregard extenuating circumstances and assign blame solely on
the effects of an act.
Piaget told his subjects two stories and asked them
to judge which protagonist should be punished more severely for his offense. In
one story, a boy named John was called to the dinner table. He came
immediately, but as he entered the dining room, he knocked over a tea cart
that, unknown to him, had been placed directly behind the floor. Fifteen cups
were broken as a result of the collision. The boy in the other story, Henry,
had been forbidden to take any jam. When his mother left the room, however,
Henry climbed up to the cupboard where the jam had been placed and, in his
attempt to get the jam, knocked a cup to the floor and broke it.
Young children (under seven years of age),
Piaget found, tend to focus on the objective aspects of a situation, that
is, on the number of broken cups.
Since John broke fifteen while Henry broke only one, they feel that John should
be punished more severely. Older
children (nine and over), however, are more concerned about subjective
responsibility, that is, with a person’s reasons for acting. In Piaget’s
study the older children felt that Henry should receive the greater punishment
because his offense was committed while he was in the act of disobeyong an
order, whereas John’s offense was purely accidental.
In a
related experiment, Elaine Walster (1966) asked undergraduates to listen to one
of several taped versions of a story and to ascribe blame on the basis of the
facts provided. The story dealt with a car that had been parked on a hill and
had accidentally rolled down. In one version, the car struck a tree, incurring
little damage. In another, the car missed the tree and struck another vehicle,
damaging its bumper. In a third version, the car rolled down the hill and into
a shop, seriously injuring a small child and the shopkeeper. Walster found that
greater blame was assigned according to the seriousness of the accidents. When
the damage was minimal, the subjects did not feel that the car’s owner was
particularly blameworthy—accidents do happen, after all. However, when people
were injured by the runaway car, the subjects felt that the owner should be
punished for not having had his brakes checked recently. Thus, it appears that
adult respondents also judge guilt, at least in part, on the basis of the effects
of acts.
(Source: Raven, Bertram H., Rubin, Jeffrey Z. 1983. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2nd Edition. United States of America: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.)
× 『rui@96yR』【butterflyuu】 ×
Komentar
Posting Komentar