Evidence on Egocentrism in Negotiation
Walster, Walster, and
Berscheid (1978) proposed that parties’ interest in fairness is not purely
objective, but that people may tend to overweigh the interpretations or
fairness rules that favor themselves (see Diekmann, Samuels, Ross, &
Bazerman, 1997). The result is that even though people display a preference for
fairness, the desire for fairness in negotiated outcomes is easily biased in
their own favor (for recent reviews, see Babcock & Loewenstein, 1997;
Wade-Benzoni, Tenbrunsel, & Bazerman, 1997). This self-serving bias in assessment or interpretation has been
referred to in the literature as egocentrism.
Thompson and Loewenstein (1992) found evidence of egocentrism
in reports of fairness, and that egocentrism reduced negotiators’ ability to
come to agreement. In their first experiment, a negotiation over wages,
participants played the role of either management or union. Participants in
both roles prepared with the same case information. Before the negotiation, but
after receiving their role assignments, parties were asked what they believed a
fair outcome to be. These estimates were egocentrically biased: representatives
of management tended to report that a lower wage was fairer. Parties then
proceeded to trade bids until they would go on strike. Thompson and Loewenstein
found that the amount of egocentric bias displayed in these pre-negotiation
assessments of fairness predicted the length of time it would take parties to
reach agreement. The greater egocentricity seen in the partie’s ex ante perceptions of fairness, the
longer strikes tended to last. (See also replications by Babcock, Loewenstein,
Issacharoof, & Camerer, 1995; Camerer & Loewenstein, 1993; Loewenstein
et al., 1993.)
In their second experiment, Thompson and Loewenstein varied
the amount of information provided to participants. Some participants received
only the bare facts about the case, whereas others received detailed background
information that had been rated as “neutral” in pre-testing. It might be
expected that more information should reduce uncertainty and facilitate
agreement (Priest & Klein, 1984). However, Thompson and Loewenstein found
that more information is associated with greater egocentrism. Those
participants who received the background information tended to make more
extreme self-serving estimates of a fair outcome (see also Camerer &
Loewenstein, 1993). Furthermore, when participants were later tested for recall
of the background information, they showed a self-serving recall bias in their
tendency to best remember those facts that favored themselves. This
self-serving recall effect has been replicated elsewhere (Camerer &
Loewenstein, 1993; Loewenstein et al., 1993), and suggests that memory biases
contribute to egocentrism during either encoding or retrieval.
Babcock
et al. (1995) provided a clever demonstration of the biasing effect of
self-interest on the encoding of information. The investigators varied the
point at which the participants in the experiment learned which negotiation
role they were to play. In particular, all participants received the same case
background information, but some read it knowing their roles while others read
it without knowing their roles. The results of this subtle manipulation were
dramatic. Parties who know their roles before they read the case materials are
four times as likely to reach impasse as are dyads who do not know their roles
when they read that information. Those who know their roles from the outset
also are significantly more egocentric both in their estimations of a fair
solution and in their predictions of what a judge will determine to be the just
outcome.
Participants
in the studies cited here did not make their fairness judgments public. As
such, these judgments could not be expected by the parties to influence the
other participants’ negotiation behavior and thus were unlikely to be
“strategic misrepresentations.” However, two studies (Babcock et al., 1995;
Loewenstein et al., 1993) offered a specific incentive for participants to be
accurate in these private fairness judgments. Participants were told that the
individual whose fairness assessments came closest to the determinations of an
objective third party would be given an extra cash award. This incentive did
not eliminate egocentrism in participants’ interpretations of fairness,
suggesting that their fairness reports reflect actual beliefs.
Wade-Benzoni,
Tenbrunsel, and Bazerman (1996) extended this work on egocentrism to a
four-party social dilemma. All participants in their experiment were given
identical information to prepare for the negotiation and were asked to report,
both before and after negotiation, what they believed to be a fair allocation
of limited resources among the four parties. The investigators report two important
findings. First, communication reduces egocentrism, a result that replicated
Thompson and Loewenstein’s (1992) finding that disputants’ interpretations of
fairness are significantly closer after negotiation than before. Second,
asymmetry in available payoffs increases egocentrism. When the four parties
face identical payoffs, they tend to share common perceptions of fairness; when
payoffs are varied among the four parties, perceptions of fairness are
divergent. This finding replicates the highly consistent pattern observed
elsewhere (Babcock & Olson, 1992; Camerer & Loewenstein, 1993;
Diekmann, 1997; Diekmann et al., 1997; Messick & Sentis, 1983) that
ambiguity in problem solving creates an opening in the decision-making process
in which egocentrism can develop via differential interpretation of the facts
and application of the relevant fairness rules.
(Source: Brewer, Marilynn B. & Hewstone, Miles. 2004. Applied Social
Psychology. UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd. pg. 277-279)
× 『rui@96yR』【butterflyuu】 ×
Komentar
Posting Komentar