The Foot-in-The-Door Phenomenon
Can we
not all recall times when, after agreeing to help out with a project or to join
an organization, we eventually ended up far more involved than we ever
intended, vowing that in the future we would say no to such requests?
How
does this happen? Experiments suggest that if you want people to do a big favor
for you, a good technique is to get them to do a small favor first. In the
best-known demonstration of this foot-in-the-door
principle, Jonathan Freedman and Scott Fraser (1966) found that after complying with a small request (for example, to sign a safe-driving petition), California
homemakers were three times more likely to comply later with a bigger request
to place an ugly “Drive Carefully” sign in their front yards than were women
had not first been approached for the small favor.
Other
researchers have confirmed Freedman and Fraser’s finding (DeJong, 1979).
Several of these studies have tried to elicit altruistic acts, such as
contributing to a charity. For example, Patricia Pliner and her collaborators
(1974) found 46 percent of Toronto suburbanites willing to contribute to the
Cancer Society when approached directly. Others, asked a day ahead to wear a
lapel pin publicizing the drive (which all agreed to do), were nearly twice as
likely to donate when the Cancer Society came calling.
However,
the seductive power of the foot-in-the-door phenomenon has limits. When the
behavior is costly to us, such as giving blood when we have never before done
so, our simply agreeing to display a publicity poster for the upcoming blood
drive seems not to markedly increase our later willingness to donate (Cialdini
& Ascani, 1976; Foss & Dempsey, 1979). But when we agree to a series of
escalating commitments, we will sometimes consent to a very substantial
request. Shalom Schwartz (1970) of the University of Wisconsin was pleasantly
surprised to find that of 144 blood donors he invited to be on call as bone
marrow donors (requiring an extraction procedure under general anesthesia with
subsequent soreness), 59 percent agreed. Schwartz surmised that his procedure
of escalating commitments had produced a “momentum of compliance.” All 144 had
first agreed to give blood and, when approached in the canteen after donating,
had further agreed to leave their seats and talk with the interviewer. After
the interviewer explained the procedure and the need, he simply asked the
people to permit their already donated blood to be tested to determine whether
their type of bone marrow was currently needed. Ninety-five percent of them
agreed to the test. Then the interviewer explained, “Carrying out these tests
is a pretty complicated and expensive business for us in the lab, so we don’t
want to go ahead with them unless we know there is at least a 50/50 chance you
will consider donating marrow if you turn out to be compatible.” Eighty-three
percent of the initial group now indicated there was a 50-50 chance that they
would donate. Only after making these commitments were they asked if they would
join “a pool of people willing to be on call should they be needed to donate marrow.”
Note
that in all these experiments the initial compliance—signing a petition,
wearing a lapel pin, agreeing to a blood test—was voluntary, never coerced by
threat or bribe. We will see again and again that when people bind themselves
to public behaviors and perceive
these acts to be their own doing, they come to believe more strongly in what
they have done. Why does this happen? The answer is not entirely clear. What,
for instance, produces the foot-in-the-door effect? One possibility is that the
initial act affects the person’s attitude
toward such action. If this occurs, then shouldn’t we expect the greatest
compliance when the small request corresponds closely to the large request?
Robert
Cialdini and his collaborators (1978) demonstrated the powerful effect of a
commitment that is similar to the desired action. They experimented with the low-ball technique, a tactic reportedly
used by some new-car dealers. After the customer agrees to buy a new car
because of its extremely good price and begins completing the sales forms, the
salesperson removes the price advantage by charging for options the customer
thought were included, or by checking with the boss who disallows the deal
because “we’d be losing money.” Folklore has it hat more customers will stick
with their purchase, even at the higher price, than would have agreed if the
full price had been revealed at the outset. Cialdini and his collaborators
found that this technique does indeed work. For example, when introductory
psychology students were invited to participate in an experiment at 7:00A.M.,
only 24 percent showed up. But if they first agreed to participate without
knowing the time and only then were asked if they would participate at
7:00A.M., 53 percent came. New experiments with University of Missouri-Columbia
students by Jerry Burger and Richard Petty (1981) indicate that the low-ball
technique is effective partly because once a commitment has been made, one
feels obligated to the requester.
An even
more popular explanation for the foot-in-the-door phenomenon presumes a change
not in the person’s attitudes, but in the person’s self-image (DeJong, 1981;
Rittle, 1981). To paraphrase Freedman and Fraser, after Melinda signs the
safe-driving petition, she may become, in her own eyes, a person who agrees to
requests made by strangers, who takes action on things she believes in, who
helps a good cause. If this “self-perception” explanation is correct, then
shouldn’t people who refuse an
initial request be less likely to
comply with a subsequent request, assuming they now feel more like nondoers? Such is precisely what Mark
Snyder and Michael Cunningham (1975) found. When Minneapolis residents were
asked if they would be willing to participate in a thirty-question survey, 33
percent agreed to do so. Others first received a preliminary call from someone
else, asking if they would participate in an eight-question survey. Nearly all
said yes, and when later the actual survey taker called and indicated the
survey would be thirty questions, 52
percent were still willing to participate. A third group of people were
initially asked if they would be willing to answer a fifty-question survey. Most
refused, and thus, when a survey
taker called with the thirty-question survey, only 22 percent were willing to
participate.
The
foot-in-the-door phenomenon is well worth being aware of so that we won’t be
naively vulnerable to it. Someone trying to seduce us, financially,
politically, or sexually, usually will try to create a momentum of compliance.
As we shall see, great evils sometimes result from the corrupting effects of
gradually escalating commitments. Doing a small evil act makes the next evil
act easier. To paraphrase another of La Rochefoucauld’s Maxims (1665), it is not so difficult to find one who has never
succumbed to a given temptation as to find one who has succumbed only once.
Marketing
researchers have found that this principle works even when we are aware of a
profit motive; hence, salespeople are often trained to use it (Reingen &
Kernam, 1977; Varela, 1971). Our harmless initial commitment—returning a card
for more information and a free gift, simply agreeing to attend a meeting just
to hear about an investment possibility—often starts us toward a larger
commitment.
The day
after I wrote the last sentence above, a life insurance salesperson came to my
office and offered a thorough analysis of our family’s financial situation.
After finishing his presentation, he did not ask whether I wished to buy his
life insurance, or even whether I wished to engage his free service. His
question was instead a small foot-in-the-door, one carefully calculated to
elicit my agreement: Did I think people should have such information about
their financial situation?
The
foot-in-the-door is not always so harmless. This process of step-by-step
commitment, of spiraling action and attitude, contributed to the escalation of
the Vietnam war. Once difficult decisions were made and defended, our leaders
seemed blind to information incompatible with their acts. They noticed and
remembered comments that harmonized with their actions, but ignored or
dismissed information that undermined their assumptions. As Ralph White (1971)
put it, “There was a tendency, when actions were out of line with ideas, for
decision-makers to align their ideas with their actions.”
[Foot-in-the-door phenomenon: The tendency for people who have first
agreed to a small request to comply later with a larger request.]
[Low-ball technique: A technique for getting people to agree to
do something. People who have agreed to (but have not yet performed) an initial
request are more likely to comply when the requester makes the request more
costly than are people who are approached onlu with the costly request.]
(Source: Myers, David G. 1983. Social Psychology. United States of America: McGraw-Hill, Inc.)
× 『rui@96yR』【butterflyuu】 ×
Komentar
Posting Komentar