Forming Impressions of Others: “Warm” and “Cold” as Central Traits
We tend to think that people separate into two groups, between warm or liked and cold or unliked. This article will explain about it for more, and also offer an experiment about the matter by Asch.
It could be argued that Asch found these sharp differences
in meaning only because the two “authors” (Jefferson and Lenin) represented
such prominent, polar opposites in the minds of his subjects. However, these
clearcut differences in perceived meaning emerged even when far more subtle
cues were used. Asch (1946) presented each of two groups of subjects with a
list of character traits describing a single hypothetical individual, and he
asked the subjects to write a brief sketch giving their impressions of this
person and rating their impressions on a series of adjective scales. For one
group, the list of traits was as follows:
intelligent-skillful-industrious-warm-determined-practical-cautious. For the
other group, the traits were:
intelligent-skillful-industrious-cold-determined-practical-cautious. Notice
that the two lists are identical, except for the words “warm” and “cold.” Asch
found that the subjects were able to take this simple list of discrete traits
and form an integrated impression of the hypothetical individual. Moreover, the
impressions formed by the two groups turned out to be dramatically different.
Those subjects who had received the “warm” list tended to rate the individual as generous, happy, good-natured, humorous, and humane; those who had received the “cold” list rated the person as ungenerous, unhappy, unstable, humorless, and ruthless. Thus, a subtle difference in stimulus traits produced two very different, but coherent, impressions of the hypothetical individual.
One of
the criticisms of Asch’s warm-cold study concerns its artificiality—the
subjects’ judgments were not based on exposure to a real person. Harold H.
Kelley (1950) answered this critism with a study of the warm-cold variable as
it operates in the classroom. Kelley told the students in a psychology course
at M.I.T. that a guest lecturer would lead the class discussion for the day.
Before the “lecturer” (a confederate of the experimenter) arrived, the students
were given a short, written a biographical sketch of him. Half of the students
received the following sketch:
Mr. _____ is a graduate student in the Department of Economics and
Social Science here at M.I.T. He has had three semesters of teaching experience
in psychology at another college. This is his first semester teaching Economics
70. He is twenty-six years old, a veteran and married. People who know him
consider him to be a rather cold person, industrious, critical, practical and
determined. [p. 433]
The other
students received the identical sketch, except that the word “warm” was
sustituted for “cold.” The “guest lecturer” then appeared and conducted a
twenty-minute discussion, during which the experimenter kep track of the number
of times the students asked questions or made comments. After the “lecturer”
had left, the students were asked to give their candid impressions of him on a
series of adjective scales and in a short, written descriptive statement.
Kelley
found that the “warm” instructor was liked far more than the “cold” one. Even
though the subjects were judging identical behavior, those given the “warm”
sketch rated the instructor as more considerate, informal, sociable, popular,
good-natured, humorous, and humane than those who had been told he was “cold.”
Interestingly, the warm-cold variable affected not only the students’
impressions but also their behavior: 56 percent of the students who had
received the “warm” sketch participated in the class discussion, while only 32
percent of those who had received the “cold” sketch did so.
Kelley’s experiment is important
for two reasons: first, it corroborates the findings of Asch’s earlier, less
realistic study, thereby lending greater support to the importance of subtle
cues in the development of an integrated impression; second, because it was
found that students were more apt to interact with an instructor who impressed
them favorably than with one who did not, the experiment shows that people’s
impressions have behavioral consequences that may be important.
The effects of “warmth” and
“coldness” on the judgment of other personal characteristics is also an example
of what has been called the halo effect,
a tendency to assume that a person who has some positive characteristics also
possesses others, and that a person who has a negative characteristic also has
other unfavorable qualities. Thus more intelligent people are seen as more
attractive, and vice versa (Solomon and Saxe, 1977), a younger job applicant
may be seen as more energetic (even if his or her behavior does not initially
suggest this), a lazy person may be seen as stupid, and so on.
(Source: Raven, Bertram H., Rubin, Jeffrey Z. (1983). SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2nd Edition. John Wiley & Sons, Inc: United States of America.)
× 『rui@96yR』【butterflyuu】 ×
Komentar
Posting Komentar